Computational models, educational implications, and methodological innovationsThe realm of visual word recognition
- Manuel Perea 1
- Ana Marcet 1
- Melanie Labusch 1
- Ana Baciero 2
- María Fernández-López 1
-
1
Universitat de València
info
-
2
Bournemouth University
info
ISSN: 1576-8597
Datum der Publikation: 2023
Ausgabe: 44
Nummer: 2
Art: Artikel
Andere Publikationen in: Psicológica: Revista de metodología y psicología experimental
Zusammenfassung
This article aims to provide an overview of the current status of visual word recognition research, from the main models and their current challenges, to the educational and methodological implications of studies in this field. Visual word recognition is a critical reading process that connects visual sensation and perception with linguistic (sentence, text) processing. For this reason, it has captured the interest of researchers in cognitive science. Importantly, it is particularly easy to model quantitatively and researchers have developed a number of computational models to explain the processes involved. Recent years have witnessed an increasing number of corpora in several languages, including average identification times of thousands of words, allowing virtual simulations of experiments to test the predictions of theoretical models without the recruitment of participants. Nevertheless, despite the advances achieved in the understanding of word processing, models still have outstanding questions to be answered, such as the role of visual information during word recognition, or how diacritics are represented at the letter level. On the applied side, word recognition research has also contributed to the improvement of educational techniques, such as the development of friendly fonts for different populations, along with methodological innovations in cognitive psychology, such as the use of linear-mixed effects models, Bayesian methods and multi-laboratory approaches.
Bibliographische Referenzen
- Adelman, J. S. (2011). Letters in time and retinotopic space. Psychological Review, 118, 570–582. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0024 811
- Adelman, J. S., Brown, G. D., & Quesada, J. F. (2006). Contextual diversity, not word frequency, determines word-naming and lexical decision times. Psychological Science, 17, 814–823. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01787
- Adelman, J. S., Gubian, M., & Davis, C. J. (2018). easyNet: A computational modeling software package for cognitive science, bridging the gap between novices and experts [Computer software]. http://adelmanlab.org/easyNet/
- Adelman, J. S., Johnson, R. L., McCormick, S. F., McKague, M., Kinoshita, S., Bowers, J. S., ... Davis, C. J. (2014). A behavioral database for masked form priming. Behavior Research Methods, 46, 1052–1067. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-013-0442-y
- Aguasvivas, J., Carreiras, M., Brysbaert, M., Mandera, P., Keuleers, E., & Duñabeitia, J. A. (2020). How do Spanish speakers read words? Insights from a crowdsourced lexical decision megastudy. Behavior Research Methods, 52, 1867–1882. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-020-01357-9
- Álvarez, C. J., Carreiras, M., & Perea, M. (2004). Are syllables phonological units in visual word recognition? Language and Cognitive Processes, 19, 427–452. https://doi.org/10.1080/01690960344000242
- Andrews, S. (1996). Lexical retrieval and selection processes: Effects of transposed-letter confusability. Journal of Memory and Language, 35, 775–800. https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1996.0040
- Angele, B., Baciero, A., Gómez, P., & Perea, M. (2023). Does online masked priming pass the test? The effects of prime exposure duration on masked identity priming. Behavior Research Methods, 55, 151– 167. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-021-01742-y
- Arnbak, E., & Elbro, C. (2000). The effects of morphological awareness training on the reading and spelling skills of young dyslexics. Scandinavian Journal of Educational Research, 44, 229-251. https://doi.org/10.1080/00313830050154485
- Baayen, R. H., & Milin, P. (2010). Analyzing reaction times. International Journal of Psychological Research, 3, 12–28. https://doi.org/10.21500/20112084.807
- Bachmann, C., & Mengheri, L. (2018). Dyslexia and fonts: Is a specific font useful? Brain Sciences, 8(5), 89. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci8050089
- Baciero, A., Gómez, P., Duñabeitia, J. A., & Perea, M. (2022). Raeding with the fingres. Towards a universal model of letter position coding. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 29, 2275–2283. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-022-02078-0
- Baciero, A., Gómez, P., Duñabeitia, J. A., & Perea, M. (2023). Letter-similarity effects in braille word recognition. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. https://doi.org/10.1177/17470218221142145
- Baciero, A., Labusch, M., Rocabado, F., Perea, M., & Marcet, A. (2021). ¿Por qué es tan fácil falsificar un logo? [Why is it so easy to counterfeit a logo?] Ciencia Cognitiva, 15(2), 24–27. https://www.cienciacognitiva.org/files/2021-3.pdf
- Balota, D. A., Yap, M. J., Cortese, M. J., Hutchison, K. A., Kessler, B., Loftis, B., ... Treiman, R. (2007). The English lexicon project. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 445–459. https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03193014
- Balota, D. A., Yap, M. J., Hutchison, K. A., & Cortese, M. J. (2012). Megastudies: What do millions (or so) of trials tell us about lexical processing? In J. S. Adelman (Ed.), Visual word recognition: Models and methods, orthography and phonology (pp. 90–115). Psychology Press.
- Balota, D., Yap, M. J., & Cortese, M. J. (2006). Visual word recognition: The journey from features to meaning (a travel update). In M. Traxler & M. A. Gernsbacher (Eds.), Handbook of psycholinguistics, 2nd ed. (pp. 285–375). Academic Press.
- Bar-Kochva, I., Korinth, S., & Hasselhorn, M. (2020). Effects of a morpheme-based training procedure on the literacy skills of readers with a reading disability. Applied Psycholinguistics, 41, 1061-1082. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716420000120
- Barr, D. J., Levy, R., Scheepers, C., & Tily, H. J. (2013). Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. Journal of Memory and Language, 68, 255–278. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2012.11.001
- Benmarrakchi, F., & El Kafi, J. (2021). Investigating Reading Experience of Dyslexic Children Through Dyslexia-Friendly Online Learning Environment. International Journal of Information and Communication Technology Education (IJICTE), 17, 105–119. https://doi.org/10.4018/IJICTE.2021010107
- Benyhe, A., Labusch, M., & Perea, M. (2023). Just a mark: Diacritic function does not play a role in the early stages of visual word recognition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-022-02244-4
- Bowers, J. S., Malhotra, G., Dujmović, M., Montero, M. L., Tsvetkov, C., Biscione, V., ... Blything, R. (2022). Deep problems with Neural Network Models of human vision. Brain and Behavioral Sciences. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X22002813
- Brysbaert, M., Stevens, M., Mandera, P., & Keuleers E. (2016). The impact of word prevalence on lexical decision times: Evidence from the Dutch Lexicon Project 2. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 42, 441–458. https://doi.org/10.1037/xhp0000159
- Brysbaert, M., Stevens, M., Mandera, P., & Keuleers, E. (2016). The impact of word prevalence on lexical decision times: Evidence from the Dutch Lexicon Project 2. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 42, 441–458. https://doi.org/10.1037/xhp0000159
- Buchanan, E.M., Cuccolo, K., Lewis, S., Heyman, T., Xiaolin, M.M., Iyer, A., ... Zugarramurdi, C. (2022, November). Is priming consistent across languages? Preliminary findings from the SPAML: Semantic Priming Across Many Languages. Spoken presentation at the Annual Meeting of the Psychonomic Society, Boston, MA.
- Bürkner, P. C. (2020). Analysing standard progressive matrices (SPM-LS) with Bayesian item response models. Journal of Intelligence, 8, 5. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence8010005
- Bürkner, P. C. (2021). Bayesian Item Response Modeling in R with brms and Stan. Journal of Statistical Software, 100, 1–54. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v100.i05
- Caldwell-Harris, C. L. (2021). Frequency effects in reading are powerful—but is contextual diversity the more important variable? Language and Linguistic Compass, 15, e12444. https://doi.org/10.1111/lnc3.12444
- Carlisle, J. F. (2010). Effects of instruction in morphological awareness on literacy achievement: An integrative review. Reading Research Quarterly, 45, 464–487. https://doi.org/10.1598/RRQ.45.4.5
- Carreiras, M., Álvarez, C. J., & de Vega, M. (1993). Syllable frequency and visual word recognition in Spanish. Journal of Memory and Language, 32, 766–780. https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1993.1038
- Carreiras, M., Armstrong, B. C., Perea, M., & Frost, R. (2014). The what, when, where, and how of visual word recognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18, 90–98. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2013.11.005
- Carreiras, M., Perea, M., Gil-López, C., Abu Mallouh, R., & Salillas, E. (2013). Neural correlates of visual vs. abstract letter processing in Roman and Arabic scripts. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 25, 1975– 1985. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00438
- Casaponsa, A., & Duñabeitia, J. A. (2016). Lexical organization of language-ambiguous and language- specific words in bilinguals. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 69, 589–604. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2015.1064977
- Castles, A., Davis, C., Cavalot, P., & Forster, K. (2007). Tracking the acquisition of orthographic skills in developing readers: Masked priming effects. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 97, 165–182. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2007.01.006
- Coltheart, M., Rastle, K., Perry, C., Langdon, R., & Ziegler, J. (2001). DRC: A dual route cascaded model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. Psychological Review, 108, 204–256. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.108.1.204
- Commissaire, E. (2022). Do both WRAP and TRAP inhibit the recognition of the French word DRAP? Impact of orthographic markedness on cross-language orthographic priming. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 75, 1094–1113. https://doi.org/10.1177/17470218211048770
- Conrad, M., Tamm, S., Carreiras, M., & Jacobs, A. M. (2010). Simulating syllable frequency effects within an interactive activation framework. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 22, 861–893. https://doi.org/10.1080/09541440903356777
- Danböck, S. K., Franke, L. K., Miedl, S. F., Liedlgruber, M., Bürkner, P. C., & Wilhelm, F. H. (2023). Experimental induction of peritraumatic dissociation: The role of negative affect and pain and their psychophysiological and neural correlates. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 164, 104289. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2023.104289
- Davis, C. J. (2010). The spatial coding model of visual word identification. Psychological Review, 117, 713–758. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0019738
- Dehaene, S., & Cohen, L. (2007). Cultural recycling of cortical maps. Neuron, 56, 384– 398. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2007.10.004
- Dehaene, S., Cohen, L., Sigman, M., & Vinckier, F. (2005). The neural code for written words: A proposal. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 335–341. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2005.05.004
- Dijkstra, T., van Heuven, W. J. B., & Grainger, J. (1998). Simulating cross-language competition with the bilingual interactive activation model. Psychologica Belgica, 38, 177–196. https://doi.org/10.5334/pb.933
- Fernández-López, M., & Perea, M. (2023). A letter is a letter and its co-occurrences: Cracking the emergence of position-invariance processing. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-023-02265-7
- Fernández-López, M., Gómez, P., & Perea, M. (2021a). Which factors modulate letter position coding in pre-literate children? Frontiers in Psychology, 12, 708274. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.708274
- Fernández-López, M., Gómez, P., & Perea, M. (2023). Letter rotations: Through the magnifying glass and what evidence found there. Language, Cognition, and Neuroscience, 38, 127-138. https://doi.org/10.1080/23273798.2022.2093390
- Fernández-López, M., Marcet, A., & Perea, M. (2021b). Does orthographic processing emerge rapidly after learning a new script? British Journal of Psychology, 112, 52–91. https://doi.org/10.1111/BJOP.12469
- Fernández-López, M., Mirault, J., Grainger, J., & Perea, M. (2021c). How resilient is reading to letter rotations? A parafoveal preview investigation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 47, 2029–2042. https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000999
- Ferrand, L., New, B., Brysbaert, M., Keuleers, E., Bonin, P., Méot, A., ... Pallier, C. (2010). The French lexicon project: Lexical decision data for 38,840 French words and 38,840 pseudowords. Behavior Research Methods, 42, 488–496. https://doi.org/10.3758/BRM.42.2.488
- Frost, R. (1998). Toward a strong phonological theory of visual word recognition: True issues and false trails. Psychological Bulletin, 123, 71–99. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.123.1.71
- Gabrieli, J. D. (2009). Dyslexia: A new synergy between education and cognitive neuroscience. Science, 325(5938), 280–283. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1171999
- Galliussi, J., Perondi, L., Chia, G., Gerbino, W., & Bernardis, P. (2020). Inter-letter spacing, inter-word spacing, and font with dyslexia-friendly features: testing text readability in people with and without dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia, 70, 141–152. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11881-020-00194-x
- García-Orza, J., & Perea, M. (2011). Position coding in two-digit Arabic numbers: Evidence from number decision and same-different tasks. Experimental Psychology, 58, 85–91. https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169/a000071
- Georgiou, G. K., Vieira, A. P. A., Rothou, K. M., Kirby, J. R., Antoniuk, A., Martinez, D., & Guo, K. (2023). A meta-analysis of morphological awareness deficits in developmental dyslexia. Scientific Studies of Reading, 27, 253-271, https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2022.2155524
- Gomez, P., & Perea, M. (2014). Decomposing encoding and decisional components in visual-word recognition: A diffusion model analysis. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67, 2455–2466. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2014.937447
- Gomez, P., & Perea, M. (2020). Masked identity priming reflects an encoding advantage in developing readers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 199, 104911. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2020.104911
- Gomez, P., Ratcliff, R., & Perea, M. (2008). The overlap model: A model of letter position coding. Psychological Review, 115, 577–600. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0012667
- Gómez, P., Marcet, A., & Perea, M. (2021). Are better young readers more likely to confuse their mother with their mohter? Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 74, 1542–1552. https://doi.org/10.1177/17470218211012960
- Goodwin, A. P., & Ahn, S. (2010). A meta-analysis of morphological interventions: Effects on literacy achievement of children with literacy difficulties. Annals of Dyslexia, 60, 183–208. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11881-010-0041-x
- Grainger, J. (2018). Orthographic processing: A "mid-level" vision of reading. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 71, 335–359. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2017.1314515
- Grainger, J., & Holcomb, P. J. (2009). Watching the word go by: On the time-course of component processes in visual word recognition. Language and Linguistic Compass, 3, 128–156. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-818X.2008.00121.x
- Grainger, J., & Jacobs, A. M. (1996). Orthographic processing in visual word recognition: A multiple read-out model. Psychological Review, 103, 518–565. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.103.3.518
- Grainger, J., & van Heuven, W. J. B. (2003). Modeling letter position coding in printed word perception. In P. Bonin (Ed.), Mental lexicon: Some words to talk about words (pp. 1–23). Nova Science Publishers.
- Grainger, J., & Ziegler, J. C. (2011). A dual-route approach to orthographic processing. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 54. https://doi.org/10. 3389/fpsyg. 2011.00054
- Grainger, J., Granier, J. P., Farioli, F., Van Assche, E., & van Heuven, W. J. (2006). Letter position information and printed word perception: The relative-position priming constraint. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32, 865–884. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.32.4.865
- Grainger, J., O’Regan, J. K., Jacobs, A. M., & Segui, J. (1989). On the role of competing word units in visual word recognition: The neighborhood frequency effect. Perception & Psychophysics, 45, 189– 195. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03210696
- Guasch, M., Boada, J., Duñabeitia, J. A., & Ferré, P. (2022). Prevalence norms for 40,777 Catalan words: An online megastudy of vocabulary size. Behavior Research Methods. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-022-01959-5
- Gutierrez-Sigut, E., Vergara-Martínez, M., & Perea, M. (2022). The impact of visual cues during visual word recognition in deaf readers: An ERP study. Cognition, 218, 104938. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2021.104938
- Gutiérrez-Sigut, E., Marcet, A., & Perea, M. (2019). Tracking the time course of letter visual-similarity effects during word recognition: A masked priming ERP investigation. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience, 19, 966–984. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-019-00696-1
- Hakvoort, B., van den Boer, M., Leenaars, T., Bos, P., & Tijms, J. (2017). Improvements in reading accuracy as a result of increased interletter spacing are not specific to children with dyslexia. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 164, 101–116. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2017.07.010
- Hannagan, T., Agrawal, A., Cohen, L., & Dehaene, S. (2021). Emergence of a compositional neural code for written words: Recycling of a convolutional neural network for reading. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science USA, 118, e2104779118. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2104779118
- Hasenäcker, J., Ktori, M., & Crepaldi, D. (2021). Morpheme position coding in reading development as explored with a letter search task. Journal of Cognition, 4(1), 16. https://doi.org/10.5334/joc.153
- Hinojosa, J. A., Moreno, E. M., & Ferré, P. (2020). Affective neurolinguistics: towards a framework for reconciling language and emotion. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 35, 813–839. https://doi.org/10.1080/23273798.2019.1620957
- Hsiao, Y., & Nation, K. (2018). Semantic diversity, frequency and the development of lexical quality in children’s word reading. Journal of Memory and Language, 103, 114–126. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2018.08.005
- Hutzler, F., Ziegler, J. C., Perry, C., Wimmer, H., & Zorzi, M. (2004). Do current connectionist learning models account for reading development in different languages? Cognition, 91, 273–296. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2003.09.006
- Izura, C., Pérez, M. A., Agallou, E., Wright, V. C., Marín, J., Stadthagen-González, H., & Ellis, A. W. (2011). Age/order of acquisition effects and the cumulative learning of foreign words: A word training study. Journal of Memory and Language, 64, 32-58. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2010.09.002
- Jacobs, A. M., Rey, A., Ziegler, J. C., & Grainger, J. (1998). MROM-p: An interactive activation, multiple readout model of orthographic and phonological processes in visual word recognition. In J. Grainger & A. M. Jacobs (Eds.), Localist connectionist approaches to human cognition (pp. 147–188). Erlbaum.
- Johnson, R. L., Perea, M., & Rayner, K. (2007). Transposed-letter effects in reading: Evidence from eye movements and parafoveal preview. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 33, 209–229. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.33.1.209
- Jones, M. N., Johns, B. T., & Recchia, G. (2012). The role of semantic diversity in lexical organization. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66, 115–124. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026727
- Joo, S. J., White, A. L., Strodtman, D. J., & Yeatman, J. D. (2018). Optimizing text for an individual's visual system: The contribution of visual crowding to reading difficulties. Cortex, 103, 291–301. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2018.03.013
- Juhasz, B. J. (2005). Age-of-acquisition effects in word and picture identification. Psychological Bulletin, 131, 684–712. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.131.5.684
- Kohnen, S., Nickels, L., Castles, A., Friedmann, N., & McArthur, G. (2012). When ‘slime’ becomes ‘smile’: Developmental letter position dyslexia in English. Neuropsychologia, 50, 3681–3692. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.07.016
- Labusch, M., Massol, S., Marcet, A., & Perea, M. (2023). Are goats chèvres, chévres, chēvres, and chevres? Unveiling the orthographic code of diacritical vowels. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 49, 301–319. https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0001212
- Lázaro, M., García L., Illera V. (2021). Morpho-orthographic segmentation of opaque and transparent derived words: New evidence for Spanish. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 74, 944–954. https://doi.org/10.1177/1747021820977038
- Li, X., Huang, L., Yao, P., & Hyönä, J. (2022). Universal and specific reading mechanisms across different writing systems. Nature Reviews Psychology, 1(3), 133–144. https://doi.org/10.1038/s44159-022- 00022-6
- Łuniewska, M., Wójcik, M., & Jednoróg, K. (2022). The effect of inter-letter spacing on reading performance and eye movements in typically reading and dyslexic children. Learning and Instruction, 80, 101576. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2021.101576
- Mandera, P., Keuleers, E., & Brysbaert, M. (2020). Recognition times for 62 thousand English words: Data from the English Crowdsourcing Project. Behavior Research Methods, 52, 741–760. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-019-01272-8
- Marcet, A., & Perea, M. (2017). Is nevtral NEUTRAL? Visual similarity effects in the early phases of written-word recognition. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 24, 1180–1185. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-016-1180-9
- Marcet, A., & Perea, M. (2018a). Can I order a burger at rnacdonalds.com? Visual similarity effects of multi-letter combinations at the early stages of word recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 44, 699–706. https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000477
- Marcet, A., & Perea, M. (2018b). Visual letter similarity effects during sentence reading: Evidence from the boundary technique. Acta Psychologica, 190, 142–149. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2018.08.007
- Marcet, A., & Perea, M. (2022). Does omitting the accent mark in a word affect sentence reading? Evidence from Spanish. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 75, 148–155. https://doi.org/10.1177/17470218211044694
- Marcet, A., Fernández-López, M., Baciero, A., Sesé, A., & Perea, M. (2022). What are the letters e and é in a language with vowel reduction? The case of Catalan. Applied Psycholinguistics, 43, 193–210. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716421000497
- Marcet, A., Fernández-López, M., Labusch, M., & Perea, M. (2021). The omission of accent marks does not hinder word recognition: Evidence from Spanish. Frontiers in Psychology, 12, 794923. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.794923
- Marcet, A., Perea, M., Baciero, A., & Gómez, P. (2019). Can letter position encoding be modified by visual perceptual elements? Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 72, 1344–1353. https://doi.org/10. 1177/1747021818789876
- Marinus, E., Mostard, M., Segers, E., Schubert, T. M., Madelaine, A., & Wheldall, K. (2016). A special font for people with dyslexia: Does it work and, if so, why? Dyslexia, 22, 233–244. https://doi.org/10.1002/dys.1527
- Massol, S., & Grainger, J. (2022). Effects of horizontal displacement and inter-character spacing on transposed-character effects in same-different matching. PLoS ONE, 17, e0265442. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0265442
- Massol, S., Duñabeitia, J. A., Carreiras, M., & Grainger, J. (2013). Evidence for letter-specific position coding mechanisms. PLoS ONE, 8, e68460. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0068
- McClelland, J. L., & Rumelhart, D. E. (1981). An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: Part 1. An account of basic findings. Psychological Review, 88, 375–407. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.88.5.375
- McClelland, J. L., & Rumelhart, D. E. (1988). Explorations in parallel distributed processing: A handbook of models, programs, and exercises. MIT Press.
- Meteyard, L., & Davies, R. A. (2020). Best practice guidance for linear mixed-effects models in psychological science. Journal of Memory and Language, 112, 104092. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2020.104092
- Mittterer, H. (2022). A “names-as-fixed-effect fallacy” in studies of name-based racial discrimination. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 119, e2209603119. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2209603119
- Morton, J. (1969). Interaction of information in word recognition. Psychological Review, 76, 165–178. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0027366
- Norris, D., Kinoshita, S., & van Casteren, M. (2010). A stimulus sampling theory of letter identity and order. Journal of Memory and Language, 62, 254–271. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2009.11.002
- Paap, K. R., Newsome, S. L., McDonald, J. E., & Schvaneveldt, R. W. (1982). An activation–Verification model for letter and word recognition: The word-superiority effect. Psychological Review, 89, 573– 594. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-295x.89.5.573
- Pagán, A., Blythe, H. I., & Liversedge, S. P. (2021). The influence of children’s reading ability on initial letter position encoding during a reading-like task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 47, 1186–1203. https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000989
- Pan, J., Liu, M., Li, H., & Yan, M. (2021). Chinese children benefit from alternating-color words in sentence reading. Reading and Writing, 34, 355–369. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-020-10067-9
- Paterson, K. B., Alcock, A., & Liversedge, S. P. (2011). Morphological priming during reading: Evidence from eye movements. Language and Cognitive Processes, 26, 600–623. https://doi.org/10.1080/01690965.2010.485392
- Pathak, A., Velasco, C., & Calvert, G. A. (2019). Identifying counterfeit brand logos: On the importance of the first and last letters of a logotype. European Journal of Marketing, 53, 2109–2125. https://doi.org/10.1108/EJM-09-2017-0586
- Perea, M., & Lupker, S. J. (2003). Does jugde activate COURT? Transposed-letter similarity effects in masked associative priming. Memory & Cognition, 31, 829–841. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03196438
- Perea, M., & Lupker, S. J. (2004). Can CANISO activate CASINO? Transposed-letter similarity effects with non-adjacent letter positions. Journal of Memory and Language, 51, 231–246. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2004.05.005
- Perea, M., & Panadero, V. (2014). Does viotin activate violin more than viocin? On the use of visual cues during visual-word recognition. Experimental Psychology, 61, 23–29. https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169/a000223
- Perea, M., & Pollatsek, A. (1998). The effects of neighborhood frequency in reading and lexical decision. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 767–777. https://doi.org/10.1037//0096-1523.24.3.767
- Perea, M., & Wang, X. (2017). Do alternating-color words facilitate reading aloud text in Chinese? Evidence with developing and adult readers. Memory and Cognition, 45, 1160–1170. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-017-0717-0
- Perea, M., Baciero, A., Labusch, M., Fernández-López, M., & Marcet, A. (2022a). Are brand names special words? Letter visual-similarity affects the identification of brand names, but not common words. British Journal of Psychology, 113, 835–852. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjop.12557
- Perea, M., Fernández-López, M., & Marcet, A. (2020). What is the letter é? Scientific Studies of Reading, 24, 434–443. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2019.1689570
- Perea, M., Giner, L., Marcet, A., & Gomez, P. (2016a). Does extra interletter spacing help text reading in skilled adult readers? Spanish Journal of Psychology, 19, e26. https://doi.org/10.1017/sjp.2016.28
- Perea, M., Gómez, P., & Baciero, A. (2023a). Do diacritics entail an early processing cost in the absence of abstract representations? Evidence from masked priming in English. Language and Speech, 66, 105– 117. https://doi.org/10.1177/00238309221078321
- Perea, M., Hyönä, J., & Marcet, A. (2022b). Does vowel harmony affect visual word recognition? Evidence from Finnish. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 48, 2004–2014. https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000907
- Perea, M., Jiménez, M., Talero, F., & López-Cañada, S. (2015). Letter-case information and the identification of brand names. British Journal of Psychology, 106, 162–173. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjop.12071
- Perea, M., Labusch, M., & Marcet, A. (2022c). How are words with diacritical vowels represented in the mental lexicon? Evidence from Spanish and German. Language, Cognition, and Neuroscience, 37, 457–468. https://doi.org/10.1080/23273798.2021.1985536
- Perea, M., Marcet, A., & Vergara-Martínez, M. (2016b). Phonological-lexical feedback during early abstract encoding: The case of deaf readers. PLoS One, 11(1), e0146265. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0146265
- Perea, M., Marcet, A., Baciero, A., & Gómez, P. (2023b). Reading about a RELO-VUTION. Psychological Research. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-022-01720-9
- Perea, M., Panadero, V., Moret-Tatay, C., & Gomez P. (2012). The effects of inter-letter spacing in visual-word recognition: Evidence with young normal readers and developmental dyslexics. Learning and Instruction, 22, 420–430. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2012.04.001
- Perea, M., Soares, A. P., & Comesaña, M. (2013). Contextual diversity is a main determinant of word- identification times in young readers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 116, 37–44. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2012.10.014
- Perfetti, C. A. (2017). Lexical quality revisited. In E. Segers & P. van den Broek (Eds.), Developmental perspectives in written language and literacy: In honor of Ludo Verhoeven (pp. 51–68). John Benjamins.
- Perry, C. (2023). Graphemes are used when reading: Evidence from Monte Carlo simulation using word norms from mega-studies. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 76, 419–428. https://doi.org/10.1177/17470218221086533
- Plaut, D. (1997). Structure and function in the lexical system: Insights from distributed models of word reading and lexical decision. Language and Cognitive Processes, 12, 765–806. https://doi.org/10.1080/016909697386682
- Plummer, P., Perea, M., & Rayner, K. (2014). The influence of contextual diversity on eye movements in reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 40, 275–283. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0034058
- Pollatsek, A., & Treiman. R. (2015) (Eds.). The Oxford Handbook on Reading. Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199324576.001.0001
- Pollatsek, A., Lesch, M., Morris, R. K., & Rayner, K. (1992). Phonological codes are used in integrating information across saccades in word identification and reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18, 148–162. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.18.1.148
- Raaijmakers, J. G. W., Schrijnemakers, J. M. C., & Gremmen, F. (1999). How to deal with the language- as-fixed-effect- fallacy: Common misconceptions and alternative solutions. Journal of Memory and Language, 41, 416–426. https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1999.2650
- Ratcliff, R., & Hendrickson, A. T. (2021). Do data from mechanical Turk subjects replicate accuracy, response time, and diffusion modeling results? Behavior Research Methods, 53, 2302–2325. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-021-01573-x
- Ratcliff, R., Gomez, P., & McKoon, G. (2004). A diffusion model account of the lexical decision task. Psychological Review, 111, 159–182. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.111.1.159
- Rayner, K., & Duffy, S. A. (1986). Lexical complexity and fixation times in reading: Effects of word frequency, verb complexity, and lexical ambiguity. Memory & Cognition, 14, 191–201. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03197692
- Rayner, K., Reichle, E. D., Stroud, M. J., Williams, C. C., & Pollatsek, A. (2006). The effect of word frequency, word predictability, and font difficulty on the eye movements of young and older readers. Psychology and Aging, 21, 448–465. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.21.3.448
- Reichle, E. (2021). Computational Models of Reading: A Handbook. Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oso/9780195370669.001.0001
- Rodd, J. M., Cai, Z. G., Betts, H. N., Hanby, B., Hutchinson, C., & Adler, A. (2016). The impact of recent and long-term experience on access to word meanings: Evidence from large-scale internet-based experiments. Journal of Memory and Language, 87, 16–37. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2015.10.006
- Rumelhart, D. E. (1977). Understanding and summarizing brief stories. In D. Laberge & S. J. Samuels (Eds.), Basic processes in reading: Perception and comprehension (pp. 265–303). Erlbaum.
- Rumelhart, D. E., & McClelland, J. L. (1982). An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: Part 2. The contextual enhancement effect and some tests and extensions of the model. Psychological Review, 89, 60–94. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.89.1.60
- Rumelhart, D. E., & Siple, P. (1974). Process of recognizing tachistoscopically presented words. Psychological Review, 81, 99–118. https://doi.org/10. 1037/h0036117
- Schoonbaert, S., & Grainger, J. (2004). Letter position coding in printed word perception: Effects of repeated and transposed letters. Language and Cognitive Processes, 19, 333–367. https://doi.org/10.1080/01690960344000198
- Schotter, E. R., Reichle, E. D., & Rayner, K. (2014). Rethinking parafoveal processing in reading: Serial- attention models can explain semantic preview benefit and N+2 preview effects. Visual Cognition, 22, 309–333. https://doi.org/10.1080/13506285.2013.873508
- Seidenberg, M. S., & McClelland, J. L. (1989). A distributed, developmental model of word recognition and naming. Psychological Review, 96, 523–568. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.96.4.523
- Selfridge, O. G. (1959). Pandemonium: A paradigm for learning. In D. V. Blake and A. M. Uttley (Eds.), Proceedings of the Symposium on the mechanisation of thought processes (pp. 511–529). H.M. Stationary Office
- Sellés, P., Martínez, T., Vidal-Abarca, E., & Gilabert, R. (2008). BIL 3–6. Batería de Inicio a la Lectura [Battery to Assess the Abilities Related with Early Reading Acquisition]. Instituto de Ciencias de la Educación.
- Siegelman, N., Schroeder, S., Acartürk, C., Ahn, H. D., Alexeeva, S., Amenta, S., ... & Kuperman, V. (2022). Expanding horizons of cross-linguistic research on reading: The Multilingual Eye-movement Corpus (MECO). Behavior Research Methods, 54, 2843–2863. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-021- 01772-6
- Slattery, T. J., Yates, M., & Angele, B. (2016). Interword and interletter spacing effects during reading revisited: Interactions with word and font characteristics. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 22, 406-422. https://doi.org/10.1037/xap0000104
- Snell, J., van Leipsig, S., Grainger, J., & Meeter, M. (2018). OB1-reader: A model of word recognition and eye movements in text reading. Psychological Review, 125, 969–984. https://doi.org/10.1037/rev0000119
- Snowling, M. J., Hulme, C., & Nation, K. (2022) (Eds.). The Science of Reading: A Handbook, 2nd Edition. Wiley. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781119705116
- Song, Z., Liang, X., Wang, Y., & Yan, G. (2021). Effect of alternating-color words on oral reading in grades 2–5 Chinese children: Evidence from eye movements. Reading and Writing, 34, 2627–2643. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-021-10164-3
- Tapia, J. L., Rosa, E., Rocabado, F., Vergara-Martínez, M., & Perea, M. (2022). Does narrator variability facilitate incidental word learning in the classroom? Memory & Cognition, 50, 278–295. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-021-01228-4
- Torkildsen, J. v. K., Bratlie, S. S., Kristensen, J. K., Gustafsson, J.-E., Lyster, S.-A. H., Snow, C., Hulme, C., Mononen, R.-M., Næss, K.-A. B., López-Pedersen, A., Wie, O. B., & Hagtvet, B. (2022). App-based morphological training produces lasting effects on word knowledge in primary school children: A randomized controlled trial. Journal of Educational Psychology, 114, 833–854. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000688
- Traficante, D. (2012). From graphemes to morphemes: An alternative way to improve skills in children with dyslexia. Revista de investigación en Logopedia, 2, 163–185. https://doi.org/10.5209/rlog.58701
- Trifonova, I. V., & Adelman, J. S. (2019). A delay in processing for repeated letters: Evidence from megastudies. Cognition, 189, 227–241. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2019.04.005
- Tsesmeli, S. N., & Seymour, P. H. K. (2009). The effects of training of morphological structure on spelling derived words by dyslexic adolescents. British Journal of Psychology, 100, 565–592. https://doi.org/10.1348/000712608X371915
- Van Heuven, W. J., & Dijkstra, T. (2010). Language comprehension in the bilingual brain: fMRI and ERP support psycholinguistic models. Brain Research Reviews, 64, 104–122. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainresrev.2010.03.002
- Vergara-Martínez, M., Comesaña, M., & Perea, M. (2017). The ERP signature of the contextual diversity effect in visual word recognition. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience, 17, 461–474. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-016-0491-7
- Vergara-Martínez, M., Gomez, P., Jiménez, M., & Perea, M. (2015). Lexical enhancement during prime- target integration: ERP evidence from matched-case identity priming. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 15, 492–504. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-014-0330-7
- Wagenmakers, E.-J., Lodewyckx, T., Kuriyal, H., & Grasman, R. (2010). Bayesian hypothesis testing for psychologists: A tutorial on the Savage-Dickey method. Cognitive Psychology, 60, 158–189. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2009.12.001
- Whitney, C. (2001). How the brain encodes the order of letters in a printed word: The SERIOL model and selective literature review. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 8, 221–243. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/bf03196158
- Williams, C. C., Perea, M., Pollatsek, A., & Rayner, K. (2006). Previewing the neighborhood: The role of orthographic neighbors as parafoveal previews in reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32, 1072–1082. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.32.4.1072
- Yin, D., Bowers, J., & Biscione, V. (2023). Convolutional Neural Networks trained to identify words provide a good account of visual form priming effects. Computational Brain & Behavior. https://doi.org/10.21203/rs.3.rs-2289281/v1
- Zhou, W., Wang, A., Shu, H., Kliegl, R., & Yan, M. (2018). Word segmentation by alternating colors facilitates eye guidance in Chinese reading. Memory & Cognition, 46, 729–740. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-018-0797-5
- Zhou, W., Ye, W., & Yan, M. (2020). Alternating-color words facilitate reading and eye movements among second-language learners of Chinese. Applied Psycholinguistics, 41, 685–699. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716420000211
- Ziegler, J. C., Perry, C., & Coltheart, M. (2000). The DRC model of visual word recognition and reading aloud: An extension to German. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 12, 413–430. https://doi.org/10.1080/09541440050114570
- Zorzi M., Houghton G., & Butterworth B. (1998). Two routes or one in reading aloud? A connectionist dual-process model. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 24, 1131– 1161. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.24.4.1131
- Zorzi, M., Barbiero, C., Facoetti, A., Lonciari, I., Carrozzi, M., Montico, M., ... Ziegler, J. C. (2012). Extra- large letter spacing improves reading in dyslexia. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109, 11455–11459. http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1205566109